EXPERT REPORT OF PATRICIA GURIN
Gratz, et al. v. Bollinger, et al., No. 97-75321 (E.D. Mich.)
Grutter, et al. v. Bollinger, et al., No. 97-75928 (E.D. Mich.)
THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS FOR THE EFFECT OF
DIVERSITY
For this litigation, I have conducted a unique series of analyses of existing data on diversity in higher education. This work consistently confirms that racial diversity and student involvement in activities related to diversity have a direct and strong effect on learning and the way students conduct themselves in later life, including disrupting prevailing patterns of racial separation. A critical question is why diversity should affect student learning and development of skills necessary for living in a pluralistic democratic society. Before detailing the results of our empirical work, I develop a theoretical rationale below for each of these types of outcomes.
The Critical Importance of Higher Education
Because students in late adolescence and early adulthood are at a critical stage of development, diversity (racial, economic, demographic, and cultural) is crucially important in enabling them to become conscious learners and critical thinkers, and in preparing them to become active participants in a democratic society. Universities are ideal institutions to foster such development.
In essays written at the end of World War II, which profoundly
affected our understanding of social development, psychologist Erik Erikson (1946, 1956) introduced the concept of identity and argued that late adolescence and early adulthood are the unique times when a sense of personal and social identity is formed. Identity involves two important elements: a persistent sameness within oneself, and a persistent sharing with others. Erikson theorized that identity develops best when young people are given a psycho-social moratorium -- a time and a place in which they can experiment with different social roles before making permanent commitments to an occupation, to intimate relationships, to social groups and communities, and to a philosophy of life. Ideally, the moratorium will involve confrontation with diversity and complexity, lest young people passively make commitments that follow their past, rather than being obliged to think and make decisions that fit their talents and feel authentic.
Our institutions of higher education are constituted precisely to
take advantage of this developmental stage and to provide that ideal moratorium. Residential colleges and universities separate the late adolescent from his/her past. They allow young people to experiment with new ideas, new relationships, and new roles. They make peer influence a normative source of development. They sanction a time of exploration and possibility (at least four years and, for many, the graduate years as well) before young people make permanent adult commitments.
Not all institutions of higher education serve this developmental
function equally well. According to Erikson's emphasis on the importance of
discontinuity from the past environment, higher education will be especially influential when its social milieu is different from the home and community background, and when it is diverse enough and complex enough to encourage intellectual experimentation and recognition of varied future possibilities. Going to college in one's home environment or replicating the home community's social life and expectations in a homogeneous college that is simply an extension of the home community impedes the personal struggle and consciousness of thought that Erikson
argues are critical for identity development.
The classic study by sociologist Theodore Newcomb of Bennington
College (1943) supports
Erikson's belief that late adolescence is a time to determine one's
relationship to the
socio-political world and affirms the developmental impact of the
college experience. This
study demonstrated that political and social attitudes --
what Erikson
would call the core of social identity -- are quite
malleable in late
adolescence and that change occurred especially for students to whom
Bennington College
presented ideas and attitudes that were discrepant from their home
backgrounds. Peer
influence was critical in the changes Newcomb documented. Subsequent
follow-ups of these
students, moreover, showed that the attitudes formed during the college
experience were
quite stable, even 25 years later (Newcomb, Koenig, Flacks, and
Warwick, 1967) and 50
years later (Alwin, Cohen, and Newcomb, 1991).
Writing long before the controversies about diversity and
affirmative action became
politically important or academically studied, neither Erikson nor
Newcomb was making an
explicit case for social diversity. Nonetheless, their arguments about
the significance of
discontinuity and the power of a late adolescence/early adulthood
moratorium provide a
strong theoretical rationale for the importance of bringing students
from varied
backgrounds together to create a diverse and complex learning
environment. Late adolescent
and early adult experiences, when they are discontinuous enough from
the home environment
and complex enough to offer new ideas and possibilities, can be
critical sources of
development. Racial diversity, given the significance of the racial
separation that
persists in this country, increases the probability that higher
education environments
will provide such experiences. Encountering students from different
racial and ethnic
groups enables students to get to know one another and to deepen their
own thinking about
themselves and about others.
Theories of cognitive growth also emphasize discontinuity and
discrepancy. Many
different cognitive-developmental theories agree that cognitive growth
is instigated by
incongruity or dissonance, termed disequilibrium by the well-known
Swiss psychologist
Piaget (1971;1975/1985). Drawing on these theories, developmental
psychologist Diane Ruble
(1994) offers a model that ties developmental change to
transitions, such as
going to college. Transitions are significant moments for development
because they present
new situations about which individuals have little knowledge and in
which they will
experience uncertainty. The early phase of a transition, what Ruble
calls the phase of
construction,
is especially important. People have to seek information in order to
make sense of the new
situation. Under these conditions individuals likely will undergo
cognitive growth (unless
they are able to retreat to a familiar world). Applied to the
experience in higher
education, Ruble's
model
gives special importance to the first year of college (or to the first
year of graduate
school), as this is the critical period of construction. In this
period, classroom and
social relationships that challenge rather than replicate the ideas and
experiences
students bring with them from their home environments are especially
important in
fostering cognitive growth.
In order to capitalize amply on such
opportunities for
cognitive growth, institutions of higher education must bring diverse
students together,
provide stimulating courses covering historical, cultural, and social
bases of diversity
and community, and must create opportunities and expectations for
students to interact
across racial and other divides. Otherwise, many students will retreat
from the
opportunities offered by a diverse campus to find settings within their
institutions that
are familiar and that replicate their home environments.
Learning Outcomes
Students
learn more and think in deeper, more complex ways in a diverse
educational environment.
A curriculum that deals explicitly with social and cultural diversity,
and a learning
environment in which diverse students interact frequently with each
other, naturally will
affect the content of what is learned. Less obvious, however, is the
notion that
students mode of thought is affected by
features of the
learning environment, and that diversity is a feature that produces
deeper and more
complex thinking. I refer generally to these mode-of-thought benefits
of diversity as
"learning outcomes."
It cannot be taken for granted that deep and complex thinking occurs
as a matter of
course among students in college classrooms and in the broader college
environment.
Research in social psychology in the past twenty years, in particular,
has shown that
active engagement in learning cannot be assumed. This research confirms
that much apparent
thinking and thoughtful action are actually automatic or what
psychologist Ellen Langer
(1978) calls mindlessness. To some extent, mindlessness is the result
of previous learning
that has become so routine that thinking is unnecessary. Instead, these
learned routines
are guided by scripts or schemas that are activated and operate
automatically. Some argue
that mindlessness is necessary because there are simply too many
stimuli in the world for
us to pay attention to. It is more efficient for us to select only a
few stimuli, or
better still, to go on automatic pilot -- to be what some people call
"cognitive
misers."
Psychologist John Bargh (1997) reviews both historical and recent
research evidence
showing that automaticity in fact plays a pervasive role in all aspects
of everyday life.
He concludes that not only is automatic thinking evident in perceptual
processes such as
categorization and stereotyping, and in execution of perceptional and
motor skills (such
as driving and typing), but it is also pervasive in evaluation,
emotional reactions,
determination of goals, and social behavior itself. Bargh uses the term
"preconscious" to describe automatic thinking. Preconscious
processes are mental
servants that take over from conscious, effortful thinking. He and
others (Nisbett and
Wilson, 1977; Greenwald and Banaji, 1995) show, moreover, that even
when people believe
that they have been thinking about something or that an evaluation or
action is guided by
a thought-out point of view, they are often wrong. Instead, they are
often guided by a
script coming from past experience -- from some kind of automatic
processing.
In one of the early studies indicating the pervasiveness of
automatic thinking, Langer
(1978) laid out many positive benefits that come when people can be
encouraged to use
active, effortful, conscious modes of thought rather than automatic
thinking. All of these
benefits foster better learning. Langer argued that conscious,
effortful thinking helps
people develop new ideas and new ways of processing information that
may have been
available to them but were simply not used very often. In several
experimental studies,
she showed that such thinking increases alertness and greater mental
activity (surely
something all college teachers strive for in classrooms).
Many terms are used to describe two basically different modes of
thought: automatic v.
nonautomatic; preconscious v. conscious; peripheral v. central;
heuristic v. systematic;
mindless v. minded; effortless v. effortful; implicit v. explicit.
Whatever the term,
higher education needs to find ways to produce the deeper, less
automatic mode of
thinking.
The social science literature demonstrates that certain conditions
encourage effortful,
minded, and conscious modes of thought. Langer contends that people
will engage in minded
thought when they encounter a novel situation for which, by definition,
they have no
script; or, when the environment demands more than their current
scripts provide, such as
an encounter with something that is quite discrepant from their past
experience. These
conditions are very similar to what sociologist Coser (1975) calls
complex social
structures: situations where we encounter many rather than few people,
when some of those
people are unfamiliar to us, when some of them challenge us to think or
act in new ways,
when people and relationships change and thus produce some
unpredictability, and,
especially, when people we encounter hold different kinds of
expectations of us. Coser
shows that people who function in complex social structures develop a
clearer and stronger
sense of individuality and a deeper understanding of the social world
as well.
These features of the environment that promote deep thinking are
compatible with cognitive-developmental theories positing that cognitive growth is fostered by incongruity or dissonance (Piaget's disequilibrium). To learn or grow cognitively, we need to recognize cognitive conflicts or contradictions, situations that psychologist Diane Ruble (1994) argues then lead to a state of uncertainty, instability, and possibly anxiety (see
also Acredolo & O'Connor, 1991; Doise & Palmonaari, 1984; Berlyne, 1970). "Such a state may occur for a number of reasons," Ruble says. "It may be generated either internally via the recognition of incompatible cognitions or externally during social interaction. The latter
is particularly relevant to many types of life transitions, because such transitions are likely to alter the probability of encountering people whose viewpoints differ from one's own" (p171).
A university composed of racially and ethnically diverse students (what I refer to as "structural diversity"), a curriculum that deals explicitly with social and cultural diversity, and interaction with
diverse peers produce a learning environment that fosters conscious, effortful, deep thinking. For most of our students, the social diversity of the University of Michigan creates the discrepancy, discontinuity, and disequilibrium that are so important for producing the mode of thought
educators must demand from their students. Vast numbers of white students (about 92 percent) and about half (52 percent) of the African American students come to the University of Michigan from segregated backgrounds. As groups, only our Asian American and Latino/a students arrive here already having encountered considerable diversity in their pre-college experience (see Appendix E). Thus, for most of our students, Michigan's social diversity is
- new and unfamiliar;
- discrepant from their pre-college social experiences;
- a source of multiple and different perspectives;
- and likely to produce contradictory expectations.
These are the very features of an environment that research has determined will foster active, conscious, effortful thinking -- the kind of
thinking needed for learning in institutions of higher education.
The work of higher education researcher Patricia King and
colleagues (King and Shuford, 1996; King and Kitchener, 1994) supports
this conclusion. They contend that college students (and adults for some time after college) are developing from a pre-reflective stage of judgment, when they depend on direct, personal observation or the word of an authority figure, toward more substantiated and qualified claims, and
then to an even more advanced stage, when thinking is fully reflective.
At the reflective level, students work from the assumption that knowledge is not given but constructed and that they must construct it. In doing this, they need to consider the context from which knowledge claims are made. They must think deeply and effortfully to take into account multiple points of view, evaluate evidentiary claims, and draw conclusions based on
conceptual soundness, coherence, degree of fit with the data, and meaningfulness. King further argues that social diversity -- having multiple voices in the classroom -- and the multicultural teaching strategy of presenting multiple perspectives from the points of view of race, class, and gender foster fully reflective thinking. Teaching students how to think about complex issues from different perspectives is a primary goal of higher education.
Although the scholars advancing these arguments about the
importance of unfamiliarity, discrepancy/discontinuity, multiplicity/diversity, and contradictoriness of expectations generally have not measured the explicit effect of racial diversity, some empirical research on the diversity of small working groups directly supports our claims. It has been shown that members of heterogeneous working groups offer more creative solutions to problems than those in homogeneous groups (Cox, 1993; McLeod, Lobel, & Cox, 1996). They show greater potential for
critical thinking, perhaps because heterogeneity of group members eliminates a problem termed "group think" (Janis, 1982), an organizational situation in which group members mindlessly conform.
The empirical analyses presented later in this Report directly test the theoretical arguments I am advancing for the impact of racial diversity
on student learning. All of these analyses confirm that racial and ethnic diversity is especially likely to increase effortful, active, engaged thinking when universities set up the conditions that capitalize on these positive environmental features, namely when they offer courses that deal explicitly with racial and ethnic diversity and when they provide a climate in which students from diverse backgrounds frequently interact with each other.
Democracy Outcomes
Education plays a foundational role in a democracy by equipping students for meaningful participation. Students educated in diverse settings are better able to participate in a pluralistic democracy. Democracy is predicated on an educated citizenry. Students educated in diverse settings are better able to participate in our democratic process. In this Report, I refer generally to these types of benefits of diversity
as "democracy outcomes."
In Fear of Diversity (1992), political scientist Arlene Saxonhouse details the debates that took place in ancient Greece about the impact of diversity on capacity for democracy. Plato, Saxonhouse says,
envisioned a city-state in which unity and harmony would be based on the shared characteristics of a homogeneous citizenry (though even he warned against striving for too much unity). However, it was Aristotle who was able to overcome the fear and welcome the diverse. "Aristotle
embraces diversity as the others had not . . . . The typologies that fill almost every page of Aristotle's Politics show him uniting and separating, finding underlying unity and significant differences" (Saxonhouse, p. 235). Aristotle advanced a political theory in which unity could be achieved through differences, and contended that democracy based on such a unity would be more likely to thrive than one based on homogeneity. What makes democracy work, according to Aristotle, is equality among citizens who are peers (admittedly only free men at the time, not women and not slaves) but who hold diverse perspectives, and whose relationships are governed by freedom and rules of civil discourse. It is discourse over conflict, not unanimity, that helps democracy thrive
(Pitkin & Shumer, 1982).
The theory of democracy that has prevailed in the United States is more akin to Plato's than to Aristotle's conception. It is the Republican tradition, represented by Rousseau on through Jefferson, in which democracy and citizenship are believed to require social homogeneity, simplicity, and an overarching common identity, rather than social diversity, complexity, and multiple identities. The model is the town meeting where people from similar backgrounds, familiar with each other, and interdependent through similarity and familiarity, come together to
debate the common good.
The increasingly heterogeneous population in the United States
challenges this conception of democracy. Little wonder that we are now facing cultural, disciplinary, and political debates over the extent to which our American democracy can survive with so much heterogeneity and so many group-based claims in the polity. Yet, it is clear that ethnic hierarchy or one-way assimilation, both of which call for muting of
differences and cultural identities, is much less likely to prevail in the future than in the past (Fredrickson, in press). Our students, as leaders of the future, need to learn how to accept diversity, negotiate conflicts, and form coalitions with individuals and groups if they are to become prepared to be leaders in an increasingly heterogeneous and complex
society.
Piaget also emphasizes diversity, plurality, equality, and freedom.
In his theory of intellectual and moral development, Piaget argues that children and adolescents can best develop a capacity to understand the ideas and feelings of others -- what he calls "perspective taking" -- and to move to a more advanced stage of moral reasoning
when they interact with diverse peers who are also equals. Both diversity and equality in the relationship are necessary for intellectual and moral development. In a homogeneous environment, in which young people are not forced to confront the relativity or limitations of their points of view, they are likely to conform to a single perspective defined by an authority. Without being obliged to discuss and argue with others on an
equal basis, they are not likely to do the cognitive and emotional work
that is required to understand how other people think and feel. Piaget contends that children do not grow in perspective-taking skills in their relationships with parents, because they are apt to accept rather than debate what parents say. With peers, they debate and actively confront
multiple points of view. They also have to deal with the strong emotions that such controversy engenders. It is these cognitive and emotional processes that promote the advanced morality that is so needed to make a pluralistic democracy work.
Several dimensions of development of the capacity for democracy can
be discerned from these theories. The conditions deemed important include:
- the presence of diverse others;
- equality among peers;
- and discussion under rules of civil discourse.
These conditions are thought to produce perspective taking,
mutuality and reciprocality, acceptance of conflict as a normal part of life, acceptance of difference and capacity to perceive commonality amidst the differences, interest in the wider social world, and citizen participation. Using these dimensions, I have empirically tested effects of diversity in a higher education setting on the capacity for democracy. All of these analyses confirm a positive relationship between racial diversity experiences during college and the capacity for participation in a pluralistic democracy.
Next "Gurin" Section |
"Gurin" Table of Contents |
"Compelling Need" Table of Contents
|